ANNEX PUBLISHERS

Journal of Nursing and Patient Health Care

ISSN: 2767-9292

Open Access
Research Article
Max Screen >>

Selection of Disease Modifying Treatment (DMT) Relates to Patient’s Decision-Making Competence in MS

Copyright: © 2021 Ariff S. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Related article at Pubmed, Google Scholar

Abstract

Background: In Multiple Sclerosis, more efficient disease modifying treatment (DMT) are often accompanied by higher risks and side effects. Selecting the optimal DMT demands from patients and doctors therefore a complex decision-making process weighing risks and benefits. Patients and doctors often prefer to share responsibility when making these treatment decisions, but this shared decision-making model requests decision-making competence on both sides. The aim of our online study was to investigate whether patients’ decision-making competence relates to DMT selection.

Method: 197 patients participated in the online survey, advertised by two patient organizations. Patients reported their DMT and who decided for or against a DMT: their neurologist, themselves, or both. We measured decision-making competence with two tasks from the Adult Decision-Making Competence Battery (A-DMC), the ability to follow decision rules and the consistency of risk perception. Perceived impairment of the disease was measured with Patient Determined Disease Steps (PDDS).

Results: The ability to follow decision rules varied with the potency of the DMT. Patients receiving basic DMT were better able to follow decision rules compared to patients receiving medium DMT. Patients who did not take any DMT stated more frequently that this decision was their own choice and independent of their doctor’s advice.

Conclusions: If patients without DMT decided against this treatment on their own behalf, doctors and caregivers potentially have to strengthen their effort to reach out to the patient and to ensure the decision is well taken.

Keywords: Decision-Making Competence, Shared Decision Making, Disease Modifying Treatment, Immunomodulation, Multiple Sclerosis

List of Abbreviations ADMC - Adult Decision-Making Competence Battery AMSEL - Aktion Multiple Sklerose Erkrankter, Landesverband der DMSG in Baden-Württemberg e.V. (Action of Multiple Sclerosis Diseased, county organization of the DMSG in Baden-Württemberg) ANOVA - Analysis of Variance DMSG - Deutsche Multiple Sklerose Gesellschaft (the German Multiple Sclerosis Society) DMT - Disease Modifying Treatment MS - Multiple Sclerosis GRiPS - General Risk Propensity Scale PDDS - Patient Determined Disease Steps

Introduction

Selecting the best disease modifying treatment (DMT) in Multiple Sclerosis (MS) patients depends on a careful consideration in each case. To reach a treatment decision in chronic disease, the patient-centered view favors a shared decision-making process that assigns equal responsibilities to doctors and patients [1]. From the medical standpoint, doctors should recommend the most effective DMT based on the predicted course of the disease and current disease activity while considering side effects and comorbidities. From the patient’s perspective, side effects might be more relevant, but preferences for DMT can be influenced by disease-irrelevant factors ranging from the mode of administration (injection, tablets) to the time interval (daily, monthly, every 6 month or every 12 month) to options to combine the DMT with family planning. Shared decision-making aims to combine those medical aspects with patients’ treatment preferences and personal values [2] and preliminary evidence indicates its effectiveness for long-term decisions [3,4].

Yet, shared decision making necessitates that both doctors and patients possess the capacity to take informed decisions, that is decision making competence [5]. DMT decisions are particularly complex because DMT vary strongly in their benefit-risk profile [6] and more potent DMT are typically associated with higher risks to experience serious side effects [7,8]. Accurately perceiving these risks and appropriately integrating benefits and risks are trademarks of decision-making competence [5], but heavily depend on memory abilities in healthy samples and MS patients [9-11]. Since memory is often affected in MS [12-14], the question arises if MS patients still possess the capacity to make a good DMT choice. Beyond patients’ cognitive ability to take informed decisions, patients may also differ in their willingness to take risks. In line with this idea, studies have demonstrated that MS patients have a lower risk tolerance or increased risk aversion compared to healthy control groups [15,16]. Our online survey investigated if DMT choices are linked to MS patients’ decision-making competence and risk preferences, in particularly the ability to follow and integrate decision rules and the ability to perceive risk consistently.

Further, if patient and doctor agree jointly upon the DMT decision, one would expect that DMT choice systematically varies with the major determinant of the doctors’ recommendation, that is a higher disease activity should be observed for patients taking more potent drugs. We used here a self-reported measure of MS disease severity and impairment, Patient Determined Disease Steps (PDDS) [17]. Finally, we studied if preference for shared decision making is linked to DMT and if this relation can be explained by decision competence.

Materials and Methods

Patients: 197 MS patients (50 men, 146 women, 1 non-binary; MAge - 45.8 years, SDAge - 11.6) completed the online survey between 12.4.2021 and 01.07.2021. The survey was advertised on the homepage of two German professional patient organizations (Deutsche Multiple Sklerose Gesellschaft, the German MS Society, DMSG, and its regional equivalent within the county Baden-Württemberg AMSEL). The survey link was also published on the homepage of Kliniken Schmieder Konstanz. The study was approved by the Ethical Board of the University Konstanz

Disease Modifying Treatment (DMT): We categorized the DMT into four groups following the German guideline for diagnostics and treatment of Multiple Sclerosis from 2021 [18], no DMT, basic drugs with little efficacy and little risks (interferons, glatiramer acetate, teriflunomide, and dimethyfumarate), medium-potent drugs with medium efficacy and medium risks (fingolimod, cladribine, and ozanimod), and highly potent drugs (alemtuzumab, CD20 antibodies like ocrelizumab and rituximab, and natalizumab). 39 (19.8%) patients did not take any immunomodulatory drug. 77 (39.1%) patients received a basic drug of group 1, 28 (14.2%) a medium-potent drug and 53 (26.9%) a highly-potent drug.

Patient Determined Disease Steps (PDDS): Patients rated their perceived degree of impairment caused by MS [17]. Patient indicated his or her subjective experienced level of impairment on a scale from 0-normal to 8 - bedridden.

Decision Making Competence: We measured patients’ decision-making competence with two tasks from the Adult Decision-Making Competence battery (A-DMC) [5]. In the Decision Rules (DR) tasks, patients were asked to choose between different fictitious DVD players based on predefined decision rules. The complexity of the decision rules varied between decision rules that required consideration of only one or all presented attributes. The ability to successfully complete the task was determined by the number of correctly selected DVD players. In the consistency of risk perception task, patients were asked to determine the probability of 10 events (e.g., death by terrorist attack) occurring within the next year or the next 5 years. Subsequently, the probability estimates were evaluated using the laws of probability. For instance, patients should judge the likelihood of an event occurring within the next year lower than its likelihood of occurring within the next 5 years.

Risk-Taking: Risk preferences may also influence the choice of immunomodulation. Therefore, patients completed the General Risk Propensity Scale (GRiPS) [19]. They indicated for 8 questions how much they agreed or disagreed with a statement related to personal risk-taking behavior (e.g., “Taking risks is an important part of my life”) on a 5-point Likert scale.

Shared Decision Making: We assessed the degree to which patients and their neurologists shared the responsibility for taking the DMT. This question asked if the patient took the decision alone, the neurologist took the decision alone, or the decision was made together.

Results

DMT and Decision-Making Skills: Table 1 summarizes how patients in each DMT group perceive their self-reported impairment (PDDS scores), how they vary in decision making competence and risk taking, and how they perceived shared decision making. As expected, PDDS scores systematically varied with the DMT they took, Welsh ANOVA F (3, 78.9) - 15.0, p .001, ω² - .18. Post-hoc tests revealed that patients receiving basic DMT reported the least impairments compared to patients without DMT, mean difference x - -1.95, 95%-CI - [-2.94; -0.96], patients receiving medium potent DMT, mean difference x - -1.30, 95%-CI - [-2.32; -0.28], or patients receiving highly potent DMT, mean difference x - -1.59, 95%-CI - [-2.43; -0.76]. Interestingly, taking DMT related also to one aspect of decision-making competence, the ability to follow decision rules, F (3, 193) - 3.556, p - .015, η² - .052. Post hoc tests suggested that patients who took basic DMT followed rules more accurately than patients who took medium potent DMT, mean difference x - .10, 95%-CI - [.01; .19]. However, DMT was not connected to either risk perception, F (3, 193) - 0.7, p - .554, η² - .01, or risk taking, F (3, 193) - .4, p - .765, η² - .005. (Figure 1)

DMT and Shared Decision-Making Preferences: Descriptively, most patients stated that they shared the treatment decision with the doctor or took the decision on their own and only a few patients stated that the doctor took the decision. Interestingly, patients without DMT stated that they more often took the decision by themselves. To understand this shift in shared decision making, we estimated four ordered logistic regression models that predicted shared decision making with DMT, PDDS, and the ability to follow decision rules (Table 2). Model comparison via AIC identifies a model with DMT only as predictor as the best model suggesting that patients who receive DMT more often perceive the decision for DMT as a joint decision between neurologist and patient, particularly patients with medium DMT. Perceived disease severity or a lower competence to follow decision rules does not alter preferences for shared decision making.

Discussion

Involving patients actively in treatment choices has become more popular in the past decades and thus debates about the effectiveness of shared decision making have gained momentum [3,4,20]. In chronic diseases, such as MS, shared decision making may prove particularly advantageous because if patients have thoroughly considered and accepted risks and benefits of a medication, they may comply more with the selected treatment option [20]. In our study, the majority of patients actively participated in the medical decision-making process. Only 5.6% of patients reported that their treating neurologist took the treatment decision alone compared to 48.2% of patients who shared the decision with their neurologist and 46.2% who took the decision alone.

Interestingly, patients without DMT reported more frequently to have taken the decision alone. Possibly, neurologists initially promoted DMT less to this patient group and therefore patients opted more often against this treatment. For instance, patients without DMT were on average older than patients receiving DMT. Knowing that DMT are less effective in older age groups [21], physicians may have considered these treatment options less. However, our data does not allow any inference about the advice of the treating neurologist.

Risk avoidance does not seem to play a major role in selection of DMT as neither risk perception nor risk preferences varied with DMT treatment. However, the application of decision rules differed between DMT groups. Patients taking medium-potent DMT struggled the most to follow predefined decision rules. Those patients also often decided jointly with their neurologist about the DMT. These findings suggest that shared decision making and decision aids may prove particularly useful in supporting patients with lower decision-making competence to take a proactive role in the decision-making process [22].

Unfortunately, we collected only limited information about the course of the disease in this online study. For instance, the online questionnaire did not contain any question about disease course (relapsing remitting or slowly progressive) because many patients are uncertain about the classification of their disease course. As a result, we cannot infer if patients were treated according to national guidelines, especially the ones without DMT.

Patients’ well-being should be the number one concern in any medical treatment decision. Knowing the patients’ decision competence may aid doctors to engage patients actively in the decision-making process and prevent suboptimal treatment choices. If patients are not treated with DMT, doctors may want to consider whether this decision was based on sufficient medical information and a trustful exchange of opinions and preferences with the treating neurologist or GP.

Conclusion

Patients who decide not to take any disease modifying treatment in Multiple Sclerosis state that they rely on their own judgment and less on clinical advice. This indicates that the therapeutic alliance between doctor should be intensified when communicating information about DMT to patients.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Deutsche Multiple Sklerose Gesellschaft, the German MS Society, DMSG, and the AMSEL, Aktion Multiple Sklerose Erkrankter, Landesverband der DMSG in Baden-Württemberg e.V. for publishing a link to our online survey on their website.

1Farin E (2014) Patient-centredness in rehabilitation: an overview on the current state of research. Z Rheumatol. 73:35-41.
2 Cocco E, Caoci A, Lorefice L, Marrosu MG (2017) Perception of risk and shared decision-making process in multiple sclerosis. Expert Rev Neurother. 17:173-80.
3Joosten EA, DeFuentes-Merillas L, de Weert GH, Sensky T, van der Staak CP, de Jong CA (2008) Systematic review of the effects of shared decision-making on patient satisfaction, treatment adherence and health status. Psychother Psychosom. 77:219-26.
4Shay LA, Lafata JE (2015) Where is the evidence? A systematic review of shared decision making and patient outcomes. Med Decis Making. 35:114-31.
5Bruine de Bruin W, Parker AM, Fischhoff B (2007) Individual differences in adult decision-making competence. J Pers Soc Psychol. 92:938-56.
6Wingerchuk DM, Carter JL (2014) Multiple sclerosis: current and emerging disease-modifying therapies and treatment strategies. Mayo Clin Proc. 89:225-40.
7Klotz L, Havla J, Schwab N, Hohlfeld R, Barnett M, Reddel S, et al. (2019) Risks and risk management in modern multiple sclerosis immunotherapeutic treatment. Ther Adv Neurol Disord. 12:1756286419836571
8Mori AM, Agarwal S, Lee MWM, Rafferty M, Hardy TA, Coles A, et al. (2017) A systematic checklist approach to immunosuppression risk management: An audit of practice at two clinical neuroimmunology centers. J Neuroimmunol.312:4-7.
9"Del Missier F, Mantyla T, Hansson P, Bruine de Bruin W, Parker AM, Nilsson LG (2013) The multifold relationship between memory and decision making: an individual-differences study. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 39:1344-64.
10Hoffmann JA, Bareuther L, Schmidt R, Dettmers C (2020) The relation between memory and decision-making in multiple sclerosis patients. Mult Scler Relat Disord. 37:101433.
11Hoffmann JA, von Helversen B, Rieskamp J (2014) Pillars of judgment: how memory abilities affect performance in rule-based and exemplar-based judgments. J Exp Psychol Gen. 143:2242-61.
12Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J (2008) Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 7:1139-51.
13Amato MP, Ponziani G, Pracucci G, Bracco L, Siracusa G, Amaducci L (1995) Cognitive impairment in early-onset multiple sclerosis. Pattern, predictors, and impact on everyday life in a 4-year follow-up. Arch Neurol. 52:168-72.
14Amato MP, Portaccio E, Goretti B, Zipoli V, Hakiki B, Giannini M, et al. (2010) Cognitive impairment in early stages of multiple sclerosis. Neurol Sci. 31: S211-4.
15 Glanz BI, Greeke E, LaRussa A, Stuart F, Rintell DJ, Chitnis T, et al. (2016) Risk attitudes and risk perceptions in individuals with multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler J Exp Transl Clin. 2:2055217316665406.
16Simioni S, Schluep M, Bault N, Coricelli G, Kleeberg J, Du Pasquier RA, et al. (2012) Multiple sclerosis decreases explicit counterfactual processing and risk taking in decision making. PLoS One. 7:e50718.
17Learmonth YC, Motl RW, Sandroff BM, Pula JH, Cadavid D (2013) Validation of patient determined disease steps (PDDS) scale scores in persons with multiple sclerosis. BMC Neurol. 13:37.
18B.Hemmer (2021) Diagnose und Therapie der Multiplen Sklerose, Neuromyelitis Optica Spektrum und MOG-IgG-assoziierten Erkrankungen. Leitlinien der DGN, AWMF-Register-Nr 030-050.
19Zhang DCH, Scott; Nye, Christopher D (2019) Development and validation of the General Risk Propensity Scale (GRiPS) Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 32:152-67.
20Ben-Zacharia A, Adamson M, Boyd A, Hardeman P, Smrtka J, Walker B, et al. (2018) Impact of Shared Decision Making on Disease-Modifying Drug Adherence in Multiple Sclerosis. Int J MS Care. 20:287-97
21Weideman AM, Tapia-Maltos MA, Johnson K, Greenwood M, Bielekova B (2017) Meta-analysis of the Age-Dependent Efficacy of Multiple Sclerosis Treatments. Front Neurol. 8:577.
22Ankolekar AD, Rianne F, Adriana B (2018) The Benefits and Challenges of Using Patient Decision Aids to Support Shared Decision Making in Health Care. JCO Clin Cancer Inform. 2:1-10.

Journal of Nursing and Patient Health Care

Tables at a glance
table-icon
Table 1
table-icon
Table 2
Figures at a glance
image-icon
Figure 1

Figure 1: Decision Making Competence and PDDS as a function of Disease Modifying Treatment

 

Disease Modifying Treatment

Variable

None

Basic

Medium

High

N patients (%)

39 (19.8%)

77 (39.1%)

28 (14.2%)

53 (26.9%)

Age

52.5 (13.3)

44.0 (10.4)

48.1 (10.8)

42.3 (10.4)

PDDS

4.5 (2.1)

2.6 (1.4)

3.9 (1.8)

4.2 (2.0)

Decision Rules

.69 (.17)

.74 (.14)

.64 (.16)

.72 (.15)

Risk Perception

.84 (.17)

.82 (.15)

.84 (.11)

.86 (.11)

Risk Taking

2.3 (0.8)

2.2 (0.7)

2.3 (0.6)

2.3 (0.8)

Shared decisions

 

 

 

 

   Patient alone (%)

31 (79%)

32 (42%)

5 (18%)

23 (43%)

   Shared (%)

6 (15%)

42 (55%)

20 (71%)

27 (51%)

   Physician alone (%)

2 (5%)

3 (4%)

3 (11%)

3 (06%)

Table 1: PDDS score, decision making competence, and shared decision making varying with disease modifying treatment (standard deviation in brackets)

 

Model

Parameter

Baseline

DMT only

Covariates

DMT + Covariates

DMT: None vs. Basic

1.6 (0.5)

1.5 (0.5)

DMT: None vs. Medium

2.7 (0.6)

2.6 (0.6)

DMT: None vs. High

1.6 (0.5)

1.6 (0.5)

PDDS

-.26 (0.1)

-.16 (0.2)

Decision Rules

-.07 (0.1)

-0.0 (0.2)

Intercept: Patient | Shared

-0.2 (0.1)

1.3 (0.4)

-0.2 (0.1)

1.2 (0.4)

Intercept: Shared | Doctor

2.8 (0.3)

4.5 (0.5)

2.8 (0.3)

4.5 (0.5)

Deviance

343

316

339

315

AIC

347

327

347

329

Table 2: Ordered logistic regression models of DMT, PDDS score, and decision-making competence on shared decision making (standard errors in brackets).